The Evolution of Mimicry
نویسنده
چکیده
Cryptic species have evolved camouflage, which enhances survival by decreasing their visibility and thus protecting them from would-be predators. Conversely, aposematic species have evolved vibrant colors which enhance visibility. These warning signals work by helping unpalatable, toxic, evasive, or stinging prey stand out from more favorable prey. Thus, predators learn to generalize the appearance of prey which taste bad or can inflict pain (Balogh 2005). Yet, all species with aposematic coloration do not have other unfavorable features. These species, known as Batesian mimics, have evolved to look like model species which are unprofitable to predators (Mappes 1997). Thus, Batesian mimicry enhances survival of otherwise unprotected species, while increasing the attack rate of the model species and deceiving the predator (Ritland 1991). As such, Batesian mimicry is part of an evolutionary exploitative relationship, in which the mimic derives benefit at the prey and predators expense. Conversely, Müllerian mimicry, in which two (or more) unfavorable prey species share similar physical characteristics, actually benefits all of the directly involved species. Since the co-mimics are unfavorable and similar in appearance, Müllerian mimics are less likely to be consumed by predators. The predators benefit because they do not have to suffer the consequences of consuming the unfavorable prey. Thus, Müllerian mimicry is a complex evolutionary mutualism which enhances survival of the involved prey and predators alike (Gavrilets 1997). There are many questions regarding the evolutionary pathway of mimicry which have not yet been elucidated. Two theories have been proposed to explain the pathway of Müllerian mimicry. The first theory, developed by Nicholson in 1927, is known as the two-step hypothesis. In this theory, evolution occurs first due to a large mutational change which causes a mimic to appear more similar to a model. After this large mutational change, the second step is a gradual change (Balogh 2005). Conversely, Fisher (1927) proposed the gradual hypothesis which is driven by predator generalization. This theory states that mimicry occurs from many small mutations each of which slightly increase the similarity between the mimic and the model. While eighty years have passed since these two theories were developed, the evolutionary path for Müllerian mimicry has still not been elucidated. Many other questions regarding the evolution of mimicry also still exist. For instance, the role of selective forces such as varying mortality rates among mimics and models in Batesian mimicry, population dynamics and associated ecological interactions, and the driving force for aposematic coloration have still not been elucidated. Furthermore, the role of imperfect mimicry, if any, in the evolution of mimicry has not been ____________________________________________
منابع مشابه
The diversity and evolution of batesian mimicry in Papilio swallowtail butterflies.
Papilio swallowtail butterflies exhibit a remarkable diversity of Batesian mimicry, manifested in several sex-limited and polymorphic types. There is little understanding of how this diversity is distributed within Papilio, and how different mimicry types have evolved in relation to each other. To answer these questions, I present a graphical model that connects various mimicry types by hypothe...
متن کاملPredator perception of Batesian mimicry and conspicuousness in a salamander.
In Batesian mimicry a palatable mimic deceives predators by resembling an unpalatable model. The evolution of Batesian mimicry relies on the visual capabilities of the potential predators, as prey detection provides the selective force driving evolutionary change. We compared the visual capabilities of several potential predators to test predictions stemming from the hypothesis of Batesian mimi...
متن کاملAvian vision and the evolution of egg color mimicry in the common cuckoo.
Coevolutionary arms races are a potent force in evolution, and brood parasite-host dynamics provide classical examples. Different host-races of the common cuckoo, Cuculus canorus, lay eggs in the nests of other species, leaving all parental care to hosts. Cuckoo eggs often (but not always) appear to match remarkably the color and pattern of host eggs, thus reducing detection by hosts. However, ...
متن کاملP105: Inhibition of Vasculogenic Mimicry in a Three-Dimensional Culture in Glioblastoma
Glioblastoma is one of the most common primary brain tumors (80% of patients) that has a poor prognosis due to malignancy. Glioblastoma has an annual incidence of 5.26 per 100 000 population or 17 000 new diagnoses per year and so as the population aging, the number of patients is expected to increase. There is a growing body of literature investigating the tumor microenvironmenta...
متن کاملImmune evasion and the evolution of molecular mimicry in parasites.
Parasites that are molecular mimics express proteins which resemble host proteins. This resemblance facilitates immune evasion because the immune molecules with the specificity to react with the parasite also cross-react with the host's own proteins, and these lymphocytes are rare. Given this advantage, why are not most parasites molecular mimics? Here we explore potential factors that can sele...
متن کاملDeception in plants: mimicry or perceptual exploitation?
Mimicry involves adaptive resemblance between a mimic and a model. However, despite much recent research, it remains contentious in plants. Here, we review recent progress on studying deception by flowers, distinguishing between plants relying on mimicry to achieve pollination and those relying on the exploitation of the perceptual biases of animals. We disclose fundamental differences between ...
متن کاملذخیره در منابع من
با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید
عنوان ژورنال:
دوره شماره
صفحات -
تاریخ انتشار 2008